Article

The disproportionate toll of the COVID-19 pandemic on social trust in low- and middle-income countries

Yongjin Choi1https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4085-062X, Dong Chul Shim2,*https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0413-0342
Author Information & Copyright
1Department of Infectious Disease Epidemiology, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK
2Department of Public Administration and Policy, Korea University, Seoul, Korea
*Corresponding author: Dong Chul Shim, Tel: +82-2-3290-3376, E-mail: dcshim@korea.ac.kr

© Copyright 2025 Graduate School of Public Administration, Seoul National University. This is an Open-Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Received: Oct 26, 2023; Revised: Oct 11, 2024; Accepted: Jan 24, 2025

Published Online: Jun 30, 2025

Abstract

Horizontal trust, which emerges from interdependence and shared values and norms among people of comparable social status, has been considered a crucial driver of individual health-protective behaviors and community resilience during health emergencies. However, recent data suggest the global decline in community resilience and its disproportionate impact on low- and middle-income countries (LMICs). To assess this empirically, we examined the difference in the degree of social trust, which was measured as trust in neighbors, in 94 countries between 2018 and 2020. The study dataset combined a global survey from the Wellcome Global Monitor and other country-level measures from Google Mobility Reports, the Johns Hopkins University’s COVID-19 Tracker, the United Nations, and the World Bank. Using a multilevel regression with post-stratification weights, we estimated that the global average decline in trust in neighbors until the early stage of the pandemic was 6 percentage points. This impact was disproportionately harsher for LMICs, causing the most salient drop of 17 percentage points among low-income countries, however, it was comparable among high- and middle-upper income countries. This finding implies a hightened degree of global disparities in social capital between LMICs and wealthier countries since the pandemic and calls for international collaboration and support to help LMICs address these additional social and economic burdens.

Keywords: social trust; vaccine confidence; COVID-19; multilevel analysis

Introduction

Perceptions towards others shape the collective consciousness of communities and society, influencing various social and health behaviors. Positive perceptions such as trust in neighbors, appreciating diversity, and assuming good intentions, help foster the virtuous cycle of affirmative interactions and interdependence among members, thereby incentivizing people to maintain moral and responsible choices to align with the community’s norms and values (Festinger, 1954; Festinger et al., 1950; Putnam, 2000). In contrast, having a negative perception can diminish interpersonal and communal connections, empathy and compassion, and, consequently, may lead to a neglect of social responsibility (Blascovich et al., 2001; Sibicky & Dovidio, 1986).

Social trust, which refers to the expectations or confidence that one has towards other people or society as a whole, is regarded as the bedrock of community resilience and health behaviors and has recently gained attention as a factor shaping society’s collective response to health emergencies. It fosters a collective consciousness that leads to a sense of belonging and solidarity among community members (Evans & Evans, 1977; Ji et al., 2020; Schoenfeld & Meštrović, 1989), which is crucial for disaster preparedness, community resilience, and fast disaster recovery (Cagney et al., 2016; Kim & Kawachi, 2017; Lalot et al., 2021; Tackenberg & Lukas, 2019). In societies with a high sense of social solidarity, people are more inclined to cooperate for common goals and engage in collective action, thereby reducing social isolation (Durkheim, 1897; Hart, 1967; Latinen & Pessi, 2014; Putnam, 2000; Ratcliffe & Newman, 2011). This promotes the effective dissemination of information and ensures that community care extends to vulnerable populations in areas where governmental support may be limited or absent (White et al., 2014; Williams et al., 2020).

During the COVID-19 pandemic, social trust emerged as a crucial component for mobilizing mass public participation in public health measures employed to fight the national emergency in many countries. Studies found that people who trust their neighbors were more likely to accept and receive COVID-19 vaccines (Cárdenas et al., 2023; Qin et al., 2022). Furthermore, it can promote people’s multicultural understanding, empathy, and shared responsibility for the well-being of the global community, contributing to support for coordinated international efforts to address global challenges (Lalot et al., 2021).

However, the outbreak of COVID-19 deepened existing social divisions and created new fractures within the society, potentially undermining the foundation of population-based interventions against the pandemic that rely on public participation. Recent evidence suggests a notable decline in the proportion of people who interact with or trust their neighbors in several countries during the pandemic (e.g., Borkowska & Laurence, 2021). Furthermore, other studies indicate that communities in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), possibly with weaker social safety nets, fragile economic structures, and limited capacity to collect data and evidence to design effective disease control policies, have had a harder hit by the pandemic (Alon et al., 2020; Khetan et al., 2022; Miguel & Mobarak, 2022). Consequently, these countries are more likely to experience severe disruptions in social capitals due to the health risks. However, this hypothesis has rarely been empirically tested on a global scale.

Recognizing this gap in the literature, this study examines one of the key aspects of horizontal social trust, trust in neighbors, by conducting multilevel linear regression using global survey data from 94 countries, obtained from the Wellcome Global Monitor (WGM) survey. We tracked the global rate of trust in neighbors between 2018 and 2020 to estimate the disproportionate impact of the pandemic on horizontal trust. In the result section, we report two major findings: a 6 percentage points decrease in the global average of trust in neighbors since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic and a a more severe decline in LMICs.

Literature Review

Social trust and vaccination

Social trust that emerges members' interdependence and shared values and norms is a foundation of collective responses to societal challenges and community resilience (Fukuyama, 1996; Negură et al., 2021; Putnam, 2000). When members of society engage in close interactions with each other, they are integrated into a collective consciousness, that engenders a sense of belonging and shared trust, solidarity, and common values (Evans & Evans, 1977; Schoenfeld & Meštrović, 1989). This collective consciousness leads to develop a cohesive culture and social norms that temper individual desires and aspirations for societal values or goals, thereby fostering a sense of unity and cooperation (Festinger, 1954; Hart, 1967; Hawkins, 1979; Ji et al., 2020; Smith & Sorrell, 2014). As collective consciousness intensifies individualistic tendencies can wane, making way for heightened emphasis on actions and thoughts aimed at community well-being (Latinen & Pessi, 2014; Smith & Sorrell, 2014; Wang et al., 2021a). These cultural ties incite “moral support” and momentum for collaboration, metamorphosing into a motivational force that facilitates the compliance with the recommendation from the authority and the voluntary care for disadvantaged others under emergencies (Amdaoud et al., 2021; Bonfanti et al., 2023). Therefore, the benefits of social trust lies in the formation of interdependent relationships and expectations through which members share common values and contribute to shared objectives (Hart, 1967; Hawkins, 1979; Putnam, 2000). Moreover, in communities where social solidarity and cohesion are robust, people actively engage in supportive activities for their vulnerable neighbors, such as the elderly, low-income individuals, and the disabled (White et al., 2014; Williams et al., 2020).

While the current literature recognizes various dimensions of social trust (Freitag & Bauer, 2013; Fukuyama, 1996; Stefaniak et al., 2022; Uslaner, 2008; Welch et al., 2005), it is worth noting the differing roles of vertical and horizontal trust in the context of health emergencies (Negură et al., 2021; Sztompka, 2006). Vertical trust pertains to the relationship between individuals and authorities possessing power. This includes the type of trust people place in formal institutions, such as the government, police, judiciary, and other public bodies or large corporations with regard to their past performance, behaviors and characteristics, such as representation and shared identity (Campbell, 2023; Cook & Gronke, 2005; Houston & Harding, 2013). Vertical trust helps maintain societal stability by fostering the public’s confidence in the decisions or policies of authorities and by ensuring compliance with the rules and recommendations set forth by these authorities (Cooper et al., 2008; Hammar & Jagers, 2006; Kim, 2010). Trust in government and health authorities—a form of vertical trust—has received particular attention in vaccination research, where it is considered a key factor of vaccine uptake. As people seek reliable guidance on prevention during disease outbreaks, governments and health authorities, the primary gateways to up-to-date information can play a crucial role in helping people correctly assess the risk of diseases and believe in the safety and effectiveness of vaccines (Prickett & Chapple, 2021; Siegrist & Cvetkovich, 2000; Siegrist et al., 2002). Vertical trust also pertains to the health system: people are more likely to have confidence in vaccines when they have confidence in the vaccine development process and health care providers who deliver the vaccines (Dopelt et al., 2023; Mesch & Schwirian, 2015). Consequently, during the COVID-19 pandemic, the importance of vertical trust in government and public health institutions has been emerged as the most salient driver of health-protective behaviors and compliance with public health recommendations, including vaccination as well as mask-wearing and other health protective behaviours, across various contexts (Choi & Fox, 2022; Miyachi et al., 2020; Prickett & Chapple, 2021; Yu et al., 2023).

Horizontal trust refers to mutual trust among people of comparable social status, including trust in neighbors, colleagues, family, and friends. Studies suggest various benefits of promoting horizontal trust among people in modern communities. Like vertical social trust, horizontal trust can also shape vaccine confidence and vaccination behaviors. Health-protective behaviors, including vaccination as well as mask-wearing and social distancing, are socially embedded behaviors, especially when considering that people often formulate their decisions by observing the actions or opinions of those around them (Short & Mollborn, 2015). The social comparison theory explains that people derive their subjective judgements about whether their opinions and actions are appropriate through interpersonal interactions (Festinger, 1954; Festinger et al., 1950). People often shape their decisions regarding health related behaviors by observing the behaviors and choices of those around them (Bikhchandani et al., 1998; Higgs, 2015). In other words, people may turn to their immediate social networks or close communities as a reference point for normative behavior during health emergencies. Similarly, studies generally find that vaccine uptake increases when the vaccination rate of surrounding people is higher (Romley et al., 2016; Tassier et al., 2015). These assessments are often benchmarked against the views and actions of others whom they hold in high regard, for example, social media influencers, and thereby, a strong preference for or trust in a particular group can exert pressure toward uniformity, prompting people to conform to the group’s norms (Bonnevie et al., 2020).

Such peer effects also explain other preventive behaviors and community resilience, making the importance of maintaining horizontal trust for societal responses to health emergencies (Godlonton & Thornton, 2012; Miguel & Kremer, 2004). Several studies demonstrated that the public’s decisions regarding mask-wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic and vaccinations in various contexts were significantly influenced by family and friends as well as guidance from local health professionals or opinion leaders (Lipsey & Losee, 2023; Rogers et al., 2021; Sato & Takasaki, 2019). A few studies also found that people with a higher degree of horizontal trust tended to be more acceptable of health recommendations (Haslam et al., 2005; Rönnerstrand & Andersson Sundell, 2015). Especially during crises, such as pandemics and natural disasters, horizontal trust enhances community resilience against external shocks. It helps encourage voluntary collaboration and mutual aid among its members and, in particular, for vulnerable populations (Putnam, 2000; Stefaniak et al., 2022; White et al., 2014; Williams et al., 2020). Horizontal trust also promotes compliance with social norms when confronting challenges and by providing a sociocultural buffer that facilitates adaptive recovery processes (Horwitz & Lascar, 2021; Jovita et al., 2019; Kokubun & Yamakawa, 2021; Mishra & Rath, 2020; Negură et al., 2021).

Disproportionate impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on horizontal trust in low- and middle-income countries

While national emergencies often have a devastating shock to communities, they can either strengthen or weaken horizontal trust among people (Aldrich & Meyer, 2015; Hawkins & Maurer, 2010; King Li et al., 2022). Studies exploring the positive impact of these crises on horizontal trust find that people can develop greater interdependence and experience shifts in social structures during and after such events. For example, the reconstruction process after emergencies could increase solidarity among survivors and positive interactions, such as support from neighbors, family, other communal forms of aid (Alesina & La Ferrara, 2002; Cassar et al., 2017).

Conversely, data indicate that the world has experienced widespread mistrust in institutions and heightened social conflicts and divisions since the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic (Flew, 2021). The virus spread through direct and indirect contact, leading people to avoid interacting with others. Moreover, social distancing measures, intended to reduce face-to-face interactions for the sake of public safety, unintentionally fostered feelings of emotional disconnection and isolation among people (e.g., Bland et al., 2022; Saltzman et al., 2021). In certain areas, these mobility restrictions reshaped power dynamics and incited conflicts among people and groups. For example, the economic challenges and disruptions of existing systems as a consequence of mobility restrictions during the pandemic questioned the legitimacy of governments and authorities, leading to public resistance, protests (Kriesi & Oana, 2023; Ozduzen et al., 2023), and armed conflicts in some regions (Levavi et al., 2022; Mehrl & Thurner, 2021). Additionally, COVID-19 became a source of social stigma and discrimination. In areas with pre-existing intergroup tensions, hate crimes and xenophobia against certain groups increased, such as the rise in anti-Asian hate crimes in the United States (Rzymski et al., 2021; Selvarajah et al., 2022).

A more pressing concern is the disproportionate impact of the pandemic on communities in LMICs (Khetan et al., 2022; Miguel & Mobarak, 2022). Compared to those in high-income countries, many communities in LMICs are more economically vulnerable and exposed to weaker health systems and social safety nets (Kruk et al., 2018). Studies find that governments in LMICs were less likely to make effective policy responses against COVID-19 (Gonzalez Block & Mills, 2003; Shroff et al., 2017). Therefore, the pandemic’s impact and the burden of government responses was heavier for communities in LMICs (Alon et al., 2020; Miguel & Mobarak, 2022). Furthermore, some LMICs were already facing underlying social conflicts and divisions, whether they were based on religion, ethnicity, or politics, especially where countries were already in conflict (Levavi et al., 2022; Mehrl & Thurner, 2021). Pandemic-induced societal challenges could amplify these pre-existing tensions, further diminishing positive attitudes toward others and eroding interdependence between people. In essence, LMICs could grapple with the multiple challenges of health issues that arise from the pandemic and weakening social trust while also confronting a prolonged recovery phase.

Despite the potential for harsher consequences of the pandemic in LMICs and a widening disparity horizontal trust between high-income countries and LMICs in the post-pandemic era, research on this topic has been very rare. A few studies reported mixed findings in the context of the pandemic. For example, using survey data collected during a COVID-19 lockdown in Germany in late 2020, Burrmann et al. (2022) reported a decline in people’s trust in others compared to levels in 2017 and 2018. Similarly, Borkowska & Laurence (2021) found a decrease in Neighborhood cohesion, measured with five items including trust in neighbors in the United Kingdom. The decline was particularly pronounced among economic and ethnic minorities. Bierman & Schieman (2020) also reported an increase in psychological distress among Canadian workers, attributed to heightened feelings of isolation and reduced trust in neighbors. On the contrary, Kye & Hwang (2020) observed increases in trust in society and interpersonal relationships in the Republic of Korea while reporting a decrease in trust in the judiciary, the press, and religious organizations. Wu et al. (2022) point out that the impact can vary by people’s socio-economic status, with those of higher socio-economic status experiencing positive effects, while those of lower socio-economic status having negative impacts.

These earlier studies provide insights into how the COVID-19 pandemic reshaped horizontal trust in specific countries. However, none of these studies offers a global overview, thereby failing to capture the widening gap between high-income countries and LMICs. Without understanding this aspect, we may lack a comprehensive understanding of the impact of the pandemic on horizontal trust and societal and community resilience more broadly, leading to incomplete interventions and inadequate post-pandemic support for vulnerable populations and marginalized countries.

Therefore, this study examines two hypotheses. First, we estimate the global decline in horizontal trust and examine the disproportionate impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on horizontal trust globally, using global survey data. We hypothesize that the pandemic’s disproportionate impact on these countries may have accelerated this erosion of horizontal trust, leading to more severe effects in LMICs compared to their higher-income counterparts. Additionally, we examine the association between horizontal trust and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance to highlight that horizontal trust was a meaningful driver of individual vaccination decisions during the pandemic. We use trust in neighbors as a proxy measure of horizontal trust in community. Trust in people with comparable social status, such as colleagues, friends, and neighbors, is a key indicator of positive interactions with others in society and has been often used to measure horizontal trust in earlier studies across various fields (Burrmann et al., 2022; Candelo et al., 2023; Lelieveldt, 2004; Muurinen et al., 2014; Zizumbo-Colunga, 2019). At the societal level, trust in neighbors can specifically capture cooperative behavior and compliance with social norms (Lelieveldt, 2004; Zizumbo-Colunga, 2019). For example, according to a study conducted in Mexico, communities with higher levels of trust in neighbors tended to have greater engagement in anti-crime initiatives (Zizumbo-Colunga, 2019).

Data and Method

This study assessed the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on horizontal trust and the association between horizontal trust and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance by analyzing global survey data with two specifications of multilevel linear regression. Our analysis aimed to utilize the hierarchical structure of the study data that account for both individual and country level variations.

Data

The study data comes from four sources: the WGM survey and country-level data from the Google Mobility Trends, the United Nations, and the World Bank. The primary data source was the 2018 and 2020 rounds of the WGM survey. The WGM survey investigated public views on trust, science, and the COVID-19 pandemic from people aged 15 and above across 147 countries from April to December 2018 and from October 2020 to February 2021 amid the COVID-19 pandemic (268,102 responses). Country-level data were combined with the WGM survey: Google mobility reports, logged population from the United Nations, and gross domestic product (GDP) per capita from the World Bank. These country-level data were obtained through the Our World in Data (https://ourworldindata.org).

As the 2018 survey did not include the COVID-19 vaccine acceptance variable, we created two datasets and conducted the imputation procedure separately: one for the trends analysis that included both rounds without vaccine acceptance (94 countries) and another for the vaccine acceptance analysis using the 2020 round excluding countries without the vaccine acceptance variable (86 countries). To create the dataset for the trends analysis, we dropped 53 countries (75,383 responses): 37 countries with a single round of data collection, 15 countries for which Google mobility reports were not provided, and one country without the income variable (see Appendix 1 for further details). For the vaccine acceptance analysis, we further excluded 8 countries without the vaccine acceptance variable. Most exclusions were due to the fact that these countries only had a single survey round, and many of them were from lower-income countries (see Appendix 2 for further details). Additionally, the WGM survey did not cover countries in regions where survey access is limited, such as the Pacific Islands.

We conducted multiple imputation to account for missing values by following the recommendation that the benefits of multiple imputation outweigh the risk of data distortion from the imputation when the proportion of missing values is over 5% (Jakobsen et al., 2017; Lee & Shi, 2021). A detailed breakdown of missing values by variable and country can be found in Appendix 2. The imputation for the trends data was conducted for 23,801 responses that contained missing values in the variables of interest: trust in neighbors, trust in government, age, gender, education, household income, logged population, logged GDP per capita, and country income categories. These accounted for 12% of the sample from 94 countries and were not missing completely at random (Little’s MCAR test: X2 (df=136)=3,759.42, p<0.001) (Jakobsen et al., 2017; Lee & Shi, 2021). We statistically imputed these missing values by including 11 variables: trust in neighbors, trust in government, age, gender, education, household income, logged population, logged GDP per capita, country income categories, and country and year dummies. The imputation for the vaccine acceptance data accounted for 10,293 responses in the 2020 survey. These accounted for 12% of the sample from 86 countries. We imputed these missing values statistically by including vaccine acceptance, pandemic-related employment changes, and the country-level mobility variable—along with the other variables used in imputing the trends data, except for the year. The number of iterations was set as 30. These procedures were conducted by using the mice package Version 3.16.0 in R.

While the sampling of the original data stratified gender, age, education, and other demographic characteristics of each country, it is widely known that data collection during the COVID-19 pandemic suffered the sampling bias due to a higher degree of non-responses (Haddad et al., 2022; Rothbaum & Bee, 2021; Schaurer & Weiß, 2020). Therefore, we applied post-stratification weights provided by the WGM survey.

Variables

The analysis examined two binary outcomes: trust in neighbors, which represents the horizontal dimension of social trust (Negură et al., 2021; Sztompka, 2006), and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance. Trust in neighbors was coded as 1 if respondents answered “a lot” or “some” and 0 if otherwise (i.e., “not much” or “not at all”) to the questions asking if they “trust people in neighborhood.” Vaccine acceptance was coded as 1 if respondents answered yes to the following question: “agree to be vaccinated if Coronavirus vaccine was available at no cost.”

The key independent variables in the trends analysis were the year dummy variable that distinguishing between the 2020 and 2018 survey waves and its interaction with the World Bank’s 2020 country income classification, which was included in the 2020 WGM survey. This classifies countries into four income groups based on gross national income (GNI) per capita: low income (lower than $1,036), lower-middle income ($1,036–$4,045), upper-middle income ($4,046–$12,535), and high income (higher than $12.535). On the other hand, the vaccine acceptance analysis used trust in neighbors as the independent variable and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance as the outcome.

Individual-level control variables included trust in government, the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on work status, gender (1 if women), age (24 or less, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65 or older), educational attainment (elementary or less, secondary, tertiary or higher), and income, which was divided into quintiles. We controlled for trust in government by considering the vertical dimension of social trust (Negură et al., 2021; Sztompka, 2006) and earlier studies that consider trust in government as one of the most salient drivers of vaccine acceptance (Choi & Fox, 2022; Prickett & Chapple, 2021). This was measured by using a 4-Likert scale question (a lot, some, not much, and none at all) and dichotomized into 1 if respondents chose a lot or some and 0 otherwise. The work impact variable was created by first recoding three work-related variables—temporary work cessation, job loss, and reduced work hours—so that a response indicating a negative work outcome was assigned 1, while responses of “no” and “not applicable” were coded as 0. A composite work impact variable was coded as 1 if any of the three variables were equal to 1, indicating any adverse work outcome, and 0 otherwise.

Country-level control variables included the mobility index, the cumulative number of COVID-19 related deaths at the end of the survey period, logged population (2020), and logged GDP per capita (2020). We constructed the mobility index as the average change in visitor numbers across four types of places, relative to the baseline period (January 3rd–February 6th, 2020): grocery and pharmacy stores, transit stations, workplaces, retail and recreation. The measures were reported daily based on the rolling 7-day average. We averaged the four measures during the 2020 survey period for each country, while coding the index as 0 for the 2018 round. Although the Google Mobility Reports also provided park visits, however, we excluded this due to its high seasonal variation—visits to parks typically peak in the summer and decline in the winter.

Analysis

We conducted trend and statistical analyses to examine changes in trust in neighbors between 2018 and 2020 and the association between trust in neighbors and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in 2020. Our trend analysis compared shifts in neighbor trust across regions—Americas, Europe, Middle East and North Africa (MENA), Sub-Saharan Africa (SSA), and Western Pacific (WP)—as well as by country and sociodemographic categories.

We then estimated the decline in trust in neighbors and the association between trust in neighbors and vaccine acceptance by using multilevel linear regression to account for the hierarchical structure of the study dataset in which individual respondents nested within countries. The first model to assess the decline of trust was:

Y ( T r u s t ) i j t = β 0 + β 1 R t + β 2 I j + β 3 ( R t × I j ) + β 4 X i j t + β 5 C j t + u j + ε i j t
(1)

where i indexes individuals, j indexes countries, and t indexes survey round. Yijt denotes the binary dependent variable indicating trust in neighbors. Rt denotes a dummy variable for the 2020 survey round, and Ij denotes country’s income category (low-income countries as the reference category). β1, β2, and β3 are the estimators of interest. The combination of β1 and β3, weighted by population size, estimates the average change in the probability of trust in neighbors across country income categories. Xijt denotes individual-level covariates, including age, gender, education, and income. Cjt denotes country-level factors, including the mobility index, the number of COVID-19 related deaths, and the logged of population and GDP per capita. The random effects uj accounts for unexplained country-level variation, while εijt denotes the individual-level random error term that accounts for residual variation within countries not explained by the model.

We then estimated the average effect of trust in neighbors on the probability of accepting COVID-19 vaccines by using the 2020 survey round based on the following specification:

Y ( A c c e p tan c e ) i j = γ 0 + γ 1 T i + γ 2 X i j + γ 3 C j + u j + ε i j
(2)

Here, γ1 captures the average effect of trust in neighbors. Xijt denotes individual-level covariates, including trust in government, the impact of the pandemic on work status, age, gender, education, and income.

We used Cramér’s V, which is more robust against the sample size than a chi-squared test in comparing categorical variables, to compare the sample characteristics between the two years. Its algebraic representation is: V=X2/nmin(k1,r1).

The analytical process in this study was conducted by using R 4.3.0 and Stata version 18 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX).

Results

Table 1 presents the sample characteristics. The trend analysis included 94 countries, while the vaccine acceptance analysis included 86 countries. Based on the imputed sample, trust in neighbors was slightly lower in 2020, compared to 2018 (79% to 72%), in the 94 countries. The percentage of people who trust government was comparable between the two years (63% in both years). Seventy-two percent of respondents answered that they would receive COVID-19 vaccines if they were offered for free in 2020. In the 2020 sample, 57% answered that they had reduced their workhour or left jobs.

Table 1. Trust, COVID-19 vaccine acceptance, the pandemic impact on work, and demographic information of the study sample in 2018 and 2020 (weighted)
Variables Without vaccine acceptance With vaccine acceptance
2018 (n, %) 2020 (n, %) Unweighted V 2020 (n, %)
No. of countries 94 94 - 86
No. of responses 96,272 96,447 - 88,414
Trust in neighbors 79.32 72.05 0.03 76.89
Trust in government 62.71 63.50 0.03 56.25
Vaccine acceptance - - - 71.76
Work impacted by COVID-19 - - - 57.07
Female 50.3 48.81 0.04 48.78
Age
 <25 22.47 24.52 0.03 25.19
 25–34 21.26 23.59 0.06 23.89
 35–44 18.41 19.56 0.02 19.59
 45–54 15.15 15.09 0.02 14.93
 55–64 11.59 8.55 0.04 8.39
 65+ 11.14 8.69 0.06 8.01
Education
 Elementary or less (8 years or less) 37.21 33.29 0.18 35.00
 Secondary (8–15 years) 50.46 51.85 0.02 50.96
 Tertiary (16+ years) 12.33 14.87 0.14 14.04
Income
 Poorest 20% 20.16 19.91 0.02 19.91
 Second 20% 20.07 19.98 0.02 19.98
 Middle 20% 19.97 20.04 0.01 20.05
 Fourth 20% 19.92 20.02 0.01 20.02
 Richest 20% 19.88 20.05 0.03 20.05
Country-level factor
 Change in mobility 0 –13.84 - –14.17
 Cumulative number of COVID-related deaths 0 76,046.69 81,622.52
 Average logged population 18.98 18.99 - 19.05
 Average logged GDP per capita 6.97 6.96 - 6.95

GDP, gross domestic product.

Download Excel Table

The 2020 survey under-sampled the low-educated population compared to the 2018 sample (27% vs. 12%), however, the post-stratification weights effectively corrected this imbalance (Appendix 3). Otherwise, demographic characteristics were largely comparable between the two rounds, and these characteristics remained consistent even without statistical imputation (Appendix 4).

Fig. 1 provides a comprehensive view of the shifts in the percentage of respondents who reported trusting their neighbors by country between 2018 (cross markers) and 2020 (circle markers). In the figure, darker markers indicate data from lower-income countries, while brighter markers indicate higher-income countries. Trust levels varied across regions and countries. Many countries in Europe, MENA, and WP regions reported trust levels above 60%, in contrast to generally lower percentages in America and SSA. SSA, America, and Europe showed a wider range of trust levels—with some countries showing very high levels and others considerably lower. Such variations likely arise from diverse sociopolitical and cultural dynamics. For example, many countries in the WP and MENA regions might be influenced by religious or cultural traditions that promote the sense of community.

jps-40-2-87-g1
Fig. 1. Weighted country-level shifts in trust in neighbors, 2018–2020. Markers indicate percentage. Circle markers indicate 2020, while cross markers indicate 2018. Colors indicate country income categories based on the World Bank’s classification.
Download Original Figure

The figure also highlights the differential impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on the level of trust in neighbors across regions and countries. Most countries experienced noticeable shift in trust levels between 2018 and 2020; some countries demonstrate an increase in trust levels, while others show a decrease. Europe had the highest concentration of countries with elevated trust levels. On the contrary, most countries in SSA and the MENA experienced substantive drops in the percentage of trust in neighbors. These declines resonate with reports of increased violence and armed conflicts in these regions during the pandemic (Basedau & Deitch, 2021; Daw, 2021).

Fig. 2 presents regional levels of trust in neighbors for 2018 and 2020, broken down by age, education, and income. The figure highlights that the SSA, WP, and MENA regions experienced sharper declines in trust compared to America and Europe. While trust declined across all age, education, and income groups in these regions, there were sharper declines among low-educated and older groups in SSA and WP. In contrast, there were only moderate changes in trust levels across America and Europe.

jps-40-2-87-g2
Fig. 2. Weighted trends in trust in neighbors by region and by demographic groups, 2018–2020. Markers indicate percentage.
Download Original Figure

Fig. 3 presents the estimated change in the probability of trusting neighbors between 2018 and 2020 (see Appendix 3 for the full regression output). On average, the probability of trust declined by 6 percentage points in 2020 relative to 2018 [95% confidence intervals (CIs)=–0.10 to –0.01; p=0.008]. The decrease was sharper in lower income countries. This impact was significantly disproportionate to LMICs. Low-income countries experienced the steepest drop—a 17 percentage point decline [95% CIs=–0.30 to –0.04; p=0.009]. Lower-middle income countries showed more moderate decreases of 12 percentage points [95% CIs=–0.16 to –0.05; p<0.001]. In contrast, the changes in high- and upper-middle-income countries were not significant (Appendices 57).

jps-40-2-87-g3
Fig. 3. Weighted changes in the probability of trust in neighbors by country income category, 2018–2020. Markers indicate the difference in the probability of trust in neighbors between 2018 and 2020 based on multilevel regression. Vertical spikes indicate 95% CIs. Control variables included but not shown: trust in government, the COVID-19 pandemic’s impact on work, being female, age, education, household income, mobility change compared to the baseline period, cumulative COVID-19 confirmed deaths, logged population, and logged GDP per capita. CIs, confidence intervals; GDP, gross domestic product.
Download Original Figure

Table 2 presents the association between trust in neighbors and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance in 2020. The result indicates that trust in neighbors was a significant determinant of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance at the early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic even when controlling for trust in government and other covariates. The probability of accepting COVID-19 vaccines was 5 percentage points higher among those who answered that they trusted their neighbors [95% CIs=0.04 to 0.06; p<0.001].

Table 2. Association between trust in neighbors and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance (weighted)
Variables COVID-19 vaccine acceptance [95% CIs]
Trust in neighbors 0.047*** [0.04, 0.06]
Trust in government 0.060** [0.02, 0.10]
Work impacted by COVID-19 0.053* [0.00, 0.10]
Education (ref: elementary or less)
 Secondary –0.010 [–0.03, 0.01]
 Tertiary or higher 0.025 [–0.00, 0.05]
 Female –0.026*** [–0.03, –0.02]
 Age (ref: <25)
 25–34 –0.024 [–0.06, 0.01]
 35–44 –0.033* [–0.06, –0.00]
 45–54 –0.012 [–0.05, 0.02]
 55–64 0.036** [0.01, 0.06]
 65+ 0.074*** [0.03, 0.12]
Income (ref: poorest 20%)
 Second 20% 0.030 [–0.01, 0.07]
 Middle 20% 0.002 [–0.01, 0.02]
 Fourth 20% –0.002 [–0.02, 0.01]
 Richest 20% 0.001 [–0.03, 0.03]
Country-level factors
 Mobility 0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 Cumulative number of COVID-related deaths 0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 Logged population 0.030 [–0.01, 0.07]
 Logged GDP –0.000 [–0.03, 0.03]
Constant 0.026 [–0.55, 0.60]
Observations 88,414

95% confidence intervals in brackets.

* p<0.05,

** p<0.01,

*** p<0.001.

CIs, confidence intervals; GDP, gross domestic product.

Download Excel Table

Other key predictors include trust in government, the impact of the pandemic on work, gender, and age. The probability of vaccine acceptance was 6 percentage points higher among those who trusted government [95% CIs=0.02 to 0.10; p=0.003]. The probability was 5 percentage points higher among those whose work was affected by the COVID-19 pandemic [95% CIs=0.002 to 0.11; p=0.004]. Female respondents were 3 percentage points less likely to accept the COVID-19 vaccine compared to other respondents [95% CIs=–0.03 to –0.02; p<0.001]. Compared to the youngest group, respondents aged 55-64 and those 65 or older were more likely to accept COVID-19 vaccines by 4 percentage points higher [95% CIs=0.01 to 0.06; p=0.006] and 7 percentage points higher [95% CIs=0.03 to 0.12 p<0.001]. Household income and the country-level factors were not significantly associated with the probability of vaccine acceptance.

Discussion

In the analysis, we examined the global impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on horizontal trust, measured as trust in neighbors. Between 2018 and 2020, most countries experienced noticeable shifts in trust levels, with the early impacts of the pandemic differing across regions and countries. Europe, for instance, maintained high levels of trust, while many countries in SSA and MENA experienced substantial declines. Furthermore, these declines were sharper in LMICs. Finally, the association between trust in neighbors and COVID-19 vaccine acceptance was found to be significant, emphasizing the importance of community solidarity and cohesion in facilitating positive health preventive behaviors during the crisis of COVID-19.

The significant impact of the pandemic on trust in neighbors, particularly in LMICs, has several implications. Disruptions in horizontal trust may indicate the erosion of social capital and community support systems that are essential for collective action and community resilience for post-pandemic recovery and future health emergencies. This may further weaken informal support networks, which are vital in LMICs, where formal support mechanisms might be insufficient. Furthermore, the contrast between the severe impact on LMICs and the relatively stable levels in high-income countries may have exacerbated global inequalities in the capacity to respond to local disease outbreaks and future health emergencies. Therefore, these disproportionate impacts underscore the urgent need for international collaboration and support to help LMICs address their unique social challenges in the post-pandemic era and rebuild horizontal trust and community solidarity for next pandemics.

These declines also open new avenues for further research on contextual drivers of the erosion of trust since the COVID-19 pandemic. Future studies could explore the role of political leadership, economic factors, cultural norms, and historical experiences in shaping horizontal trust across different communities and countries during health emergencies.

The finding that people who trusted their neighbors were significantly more likely to accept the COVID-19 vaccine suggests that declines in horizontal trust may have serious health consequences. This association supports the role of social capital, solidarity, and cohesion in promoting health and health behaviors (Cárdenas et al., 2023; Lalot et al., 2021; Qin et al., 2022; Ratcliffe & Newman, 2011) and expands the idea into the context of health emergencies at a global scale. Robust social networks and community solidarity can facilitate the willingness to vaccinate, especially in the early stage of a crisis. Therefore, the erosion of horizontal trust since the COVID-19 pandemic may leave countries more vulnerable to the threats from future health emergencies. Similarly, the strong effect of trust in government on vaccine acceptance echoes the importance of institutional trust during health emergencies. This result is consistent with earlier findings that suggest trust in government is critical for the success of immunization programs (Choi & Fox, 2022; Miyachi et al., 2020; Prickett & Chapple, 2021; Yu et al., 2023).

Age-related differences in vaccine acceptance largely align with observations from earlier studies and could be understood through the lens of risk perception. Older populations, who face a higher risk of severe COVID-19 outcomes may be more willing to protect themselves from COVID-19 by getting a vaccine. In contrast, younger age groups, who perceive themselves at lower risk, are less willing to get vaccinated. Overall, these findings contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of the factors influencing vaccine acceptance, drawing on theories related to social trust, institutional trust, and risk perception.

However, we should be cautious when generalizing the observation that COVID-19 vaccine acceptance was lower among female respondents. Earlier studies in various contexts suggest women tend to be more risk averse than men (e.g., Ferrín, 2022) but are more likely to express concerns about both COVID-19 (Alsharawy et al., 2021; Lewis & Duch, 2021) and potential vaccine side effects (Toshkov, 2023). Moreover, studies indicate that men have a lower perception of disease risk, which may contribute to a lower acceptance rate of COVID-19 vaccines (Wang et al., 2021b). Consequently, the literature reports mixed findings on whether women are more or less likely than men to accept COVID-19 vaccines (Pires, 2022).

This study has several limitations that need consideration. First, the study findings rely on cross-sectional survey data and therefore may not enable a rigorous examination of causality. Further research could improve our understanding of the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on horizontal trust by using longitudinal data or quasi-experimental design. Second, while our analysis provides global estimates and examines their variations across countries, it does not fully consider the unique circumstances of each country during the pandemic. This oversight might neglect specific local factors that affect horizontal trust and vaccine acceptance under various conditions. Third, the representativeness of the study data could be compromised by the substantial number of missing values and countries and subject to sampling bias linked to restricted participation to the survey during the COVID-19 pandemic (Brubaker et al., 2021; Lin et al., 2021). We acknowledge that excluding these countries may introduce some bias compared to including all countries. Although we mitigated the problem of missing values by employing statistical imputation techniques and the sampling bias by applying post-stratification weights, the study results might still be skewed toward certain demographic groups or countries that were excluded from the analysis. Nonetheless, the findings from this analysis remain significant, as the study still includes data from a number of countries. These results also provide a key indication of the disproportionate burdens from the COVID-19 pandemic, pointing to areas that require additional research.

Competing interests

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Funding sources

This work was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2022S1A5C2A03091302).

This work was also supported by Korea University Research Fund (K2208301).

Acknowledgements

Code that support the findings of this study is available at: https://github.com/TheYongjinChoi/jps-social-trust

Availability of data and material

The primary dataset used in this study can be accessed from the Wellcome Global Monitor:

- Wellcome Global Monitor – COVID-19 (2020): https://wellcome.org/reports/wellcome-global-monitor-covid-19/2020

- Wellcome Global Monitor (2018): https://wellcome.org/reports/wellcome-global-monitor/2018

References

1.

Aldrich, D. P., & Meyer, M. A. (2015). Social capital and community resilience. American Behavioral Scientist, 59(2), 254-269.

2.

Alesina, A., & La Ferrara, E. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics, 85(2), 207-234.

3.

Alon, T., Kim, M., Lagakos, D., & Van Vuren, M. (2020). How should policy responses to the COVID-19 pandemic differ in the developing world?. NBER.

4.

Alsharawy, A., Spoon, R., Smith, A., & Ball, S. (2021). Gender differences in fear and risk perception during the COVID-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 689467.

5.

Amdaoud, M., Arcuri, G., & Levratto, N. (2021). Healthcare system and social trust in the fight against COVID-19: The case of France. European Journal of Public Health, 31(4), 895-900.

6.

Basedau, M., & Deitch, M. (2021). One year after: Has the COVID-19 pandemic increased violence in sub-Saharan Africa? (GIGA Working Papers No. 327). German Institute of Global and Area Studies (GIGA).

7.

Bierman, A., & Schieman, S. (2020). Social estrangement and psychological distress before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: Patterns of change in Canadian workers. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 61(4), 398-417.

8.

Bikhchandani, S., Hirshleifer, D., & Welch, I. (1998). Learning from the behavior of others: Conformity, fads, and informational cascades. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(3), 151-170.

9.

Bland, A. R., Roiser, J. P., Mehta, M. A., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W., & Elliott, R. (2022). The impact of COVID-19 social isolation on aspects of emotional and social cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 36(1), 49-58.

10.

Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., & Kowai-Bell, N. (2001). Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 253-267.

11.

Bonfanti, R. C., Oberti, B., Ravazzoli, E., Rinaldi, A., Ruggieri, S., & Schimmenti, A. (2023). The role of trust in disaster risk reduction: A critical review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 21(1), 29.

12.

Bonnevie, E., Rosenberg, S. D., Kummeth, C., Goldbarg, J., Wartella, E., & Smyser, J. (2020). Using social media influencers to increase knowledge and positive attitudes toward the flu vaccine. PLOS ONE, 15(10), e0240828.

13.

Borkowska, M., & Laurence, J. (2021). Coming together or coming apart? Changes in social cohesion during the Covid-19 pandemic in England. European Societies, 23(S1), S618-S636.

14.

Brubaker, J., Kilic, T., & Wollburg, P. (2021). Representativeness of individual-level data in COVID-19 phone surveys: Findings from Sub-Saharan Africa. PLOS ONE, 16(11), e0258877.

15.

Burrmann, U., Mutz, M., Sielschott, S., & Braun, S. (2022). Social capital trends in Germany in the face of the Covid-19 pandemic: A research note. Frontiers in Political Science, 4, 814943.

16.

Cagney, K. A., Sterrett, D., Benz, J., & Tompson, T. (2016). Social resources and community resilience in the wake of superstorm Sandy. PLOS ONE, 11(8), e0160824.

17.

Campbell, J. W. (2023). Public participation and trust in government: Results from a vignette experiment. Journal of Policy Studies, 38(2), 23-31.

18.

Candelo, N., de Oliveira, A. C. M., & Eckel, C. (2023). Trust among the poor: African Americans trust their neighbors, but are less trusting of public officials. Public Choice, 196(3/4), 427-452.

19.

Cárdenas, D., Orazani, N., Manueli, F., Donaldson, J. L., Stevens, M., Cruwys, T., Platow, M. J., O’Donnell, J., Zekulin, M. G., Qureshi, I., Walker, I., & Reynolds, K. J. (2023). Social cohesion predicts COVID-19 vaccination intentions and uptake. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 17(7), e12759.

20.

Cassar, A., Healy, A., & von Kessler, C. (2017). Trust, risk, and time preferences after a natural disaster: Experimental evidence from Thailand. World Development, 94, 90-105.

21.

Choi, Y., & Fox, A. M. (2022). Mistrust in public health institutions is a stronger predictor of vaccine hesitancy and uptake than trust in Trump. Social Science & Medicine, 314, 115440.

22.

Cook, T. E., & Gronke, P. (2005). The skeptical American: Revisiting the meanings of trust in government and confidence in institutions. The Journal of Politics, 67(3), 784-803.

23.

Cooper, C. A., Knotts, H. G., & Brennan, K. M. (2008). The importance of trust in government for public administration: The case of zoning. Public Administration Review, 68(3), 459-468.

24.

Daw, M. A. (2021). The impact of armed conflict on the epidemiological situation of COVID-19 in Libya, Syria and Yemen. Frontiers in Public Health, 9, 667364.

25.

Dopelt, K., Abudin, A., Yukther, S., Shmukler, T., & Davidovitch, N. (2023). The association between levels of trust in the healthcare system and influenza vaccine hesitancy among college students in Israel. Vaccines, 11(11), 1728.

26.

Durkheim, É. (1897). Suicide: A study in sociology. In J. A. Spaulding, & G. Simpson (Eds.), Suicide: A study in sociology. The Free Press.

27.

Evans, A. Jr., & Evans, A. (1977). An examination of the concept “social solidarity”. Mid-American Review of Sociology, 2(1), 29-46.

28.

Ferrín, M. (2022). Reassessing gender differences in COVID-19 risk perception and behavior. Social Science Quarterly, 103(1), 31-41.

29.

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117-140.

30.

Festinger, L., Back, K., Schachter, S., Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. (1950). Theory and experiment in social communication. Research Center for Dynamics Institute for Social Research.

31.

Flew, T. (2021). The global trust deficit disorder: A communications perspective on trust in the time of global pandemics. Journal of Communication, 71(2), 163-186.

32.

Freitag, M., & Bauer, P. C. (2013). Testing for measurement equivalence in surveys: Dimensions of social trust across cultural contexts. Public Opinion Quarterly, 77(S1), 24-44.

33.

Fukuyama, F. (1996). Trust: The social virtues and the creation of prosperity. Free Press.

34.

Godlonton, S., & Thornton, R. (2012). Peer effects in learning HIV results. Journal of Development Economics, 97(1), 118-129.

35.

Gonzalez Block, M. A., & Mills, A. (2003). Assessing capacity for health policy and systems research in low and middle income countries. Health Research Policy and Systems, 1(1), 1.

36.

Haddad, C., Sacre, H., Zeenny, R. M., Hajj, A., Akel, M., Iskandar, K., & Salameh, P. (2022). Should samples be weighted to decrease selection bias in online surveys during the COVID-19 pandemic? Data from seven datasets. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 22(1), 63.

37.

Hammar, H., & Jagers, S. C. (2006). Can trust in politicians explain individuals’ support for climate policy? The case of CO2 tax. Climate Policy, 5(6), 613-625.

38.

Hart, H. L. A. (1967). Social solidarity and the enforcement of morality. The University of Chicago Law Review, 35(1), 1-13.

39.

Haslam, S. A., O’Brien, A., Jetten, J., Vormedal, K., & Penna, S. (2005). Taking the strain: Social identity, social support, and the experience of stress. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 355-370.

40.

Hawkins, M. J. (1979). Continuity and change in Durkheim’s theory of social solidarity. The Sociological Quarterly, 20(1), 155-164.

41.

Hawkins, R. L., & Maurer, K. (2010). Bonding, bridging and linking: How social capital operated in New Orleans following Hurricane Katrina. The British Journal of Social Work, 40(6), 1777-1793.

42.

Higgs, S. (2015). Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite, 86, 38-44.

43.

Horwitz, I. M., & Lascar, S. (2021). Ties in tough times: How social capital helps lower-income Jewish parents weather the economic hardship of COVID-19. Contemporary Jewry, 41(1), 161-183.

44.

Houston, D. J., & Harding, L. H. (2013). Public trust in government administrators: Explaining citizen perceptions of trustworthiness and competence. Public Integrity, 16(1), 53-76.

45.

Jakobsen, J. C., Gluud, C., Wetterslev, J., & Winkel, P. (2017). When and how should multiple imputation be used for handling missing data in randomised clinical trials: A practical guide with flowcharts. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 17(1), 162.

46.

Ji, L., Shun, X., Kai, L., & Amin, M. A. (2020). Nonprofit membership and interpersonal trust in diversity. Korean Journal of Policy Studies, 35(1), 53-73.

47.

Jovita, H. D., Nashir, H., Mutiarin, D., Moner, Y., & Nurmandi, A. (2019). Social capital and disasters: How does social capital shape post-disaster conditions in the Philippines? Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 29(4), 519-534.

48.

Khetan, A. K., Yusuf, S., Lopez-Jaramillo, P., Szuba, A., Orlandini, A., Mat-Nasir, N., Oguz, A., Gupta, R., Avezum, Á., Rosnah, I., Poirier, P., Teo, K. K., Wielgosz, A., Lear, S. A., Palileo-Villanueva, L. M., Serón, P., Chifamba, J., Rangarajan, S., Mushtaha, M., … Leong, D. P. (2022). Variations in the financial impact of the COVID-19 pandemic across 5 continents: A cross-sectional, individual level analysis. eClinicalMedicine, 44, 101284.

49.

Kim, E. S., & Kawachi, I. (2017). Perceived neighborhood social cohesion and preventive healthcare use. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 53(2), e35-e40.

50.

Kim, S. (2010). Public trust in government in Japan and South Korea: Does the rise of critical citizens matter? Public Administration Review, 70(5), 801-810.

51.

King Li, K., Hong, Y., Huang, B., & Tam, T. (2022). Social preferences before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Judgment and Decision Making, 17(6), 1313-1333.

52.

Kokubun, K., & Yamakawa, Y. (2021). Social capital mediates the relationship between social distancing and COVID-19 prevalence in Japan. INQUIRY: The Journal of Healthcare Organization, Provision, and Financing, 58, 1-11.

53.

Kriesi, H., & Oana, I. E. (2023). Protest in unlikely times: Dynamics of collective mobilization in Europe during the COVID-19 crisis. Journal of European Public Policy, 30(4), 740-765.

54.

Kruk, M. E., Gage, A. D., Joseph, N. T., Danaei, G., García-Saisó, S., & Salomon, J. A. (2018). Mortality due to low-quality health systems in the universal health coverage era: A systematic analysis of amenable deaths in 137 countries. The Lancet, 392(10160), 2203-2212.

55.

Kye, B., & Hwang, S. J. (2020). Social trust in the midst of pandemic crisis: Implications from COVID-19 of South Korea. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 68, 100523.

56.

Lalot, F., Abrams, D., Broadwood, J., Davies Hayon, K., & Platts-Dunn, I. (2021). The social cohesion investment: Communities that invested in integration programmes are showing greater social cohesion in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 32(3), 536-554.

57.

Latinen, A., & Pessi, A. B. (2014). Solidarity: Theory and practice. An introduction. Lexington Books.

58.

Lee, T., & Shi, D. (2021). A comparison of full information maximum likelihood and multiple imputation in structural equation modeling with missing data. Psychological Methods, 26(4), 466-485.

59.

Lelieveldt, H. (2004). Helping citizens help themselves: Neighborhood improvement programs and the impact of social networks, trust, and norms on neighborhood-oriented forms of participation. Urban Affairs Review, 39(5), 531-551.

60.

Levavi, K., Yakov, P., Pike, A., Deater-Deckard, K., Hadar, A., Bar, G., Froimovici, M., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2022). When COVID-19 met families living in armed-conflict zones: The importance of maternal trauma and child self-regulation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 718455.

61.

Lewis, A., & Duch, R. (2021). Gender differences in perceived risk of COVID-19. Social Science Quarterly, 102(5), 2124-2133.

62.

Lin, Y. H., Chen, C. Y., & Wu, S. I. (2021). Efficiency and quality of data collection among public mental health surveys conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic: Systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 23(2), e25118.

63.

Lipsey, N. P., & Losee, J. E. (2023). Social influences on mask-wearing intentions during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 17(10), e12817.

64.

Mehrl, M., & Thurner, P. W. (2021). The effect of the Covid-19 pandemic on global armed conflict: Early evidence. Political Studies Review, 19(2), 286-293.

65.

Mesch, G. S., & Schwirian, K. P. (2015). Social and political determinants of vaccine hesitancy: Lessons learned from the H1N1 pandemic of 2009-2010. American Journal of Infection Control, 43(11), 1161-1165.

66.

Miguel, E., & Kremer, M. (2004). Worms: Identifying impacts on education and health in the presence of treatment externalities. Econometrica, 72(1), 159-217.

67.

Miguel, E., & Mobarak, A. M. (2022). The economics of the COVID-19 pandemic in poor countries. Annual Review of Economics, 14(1), 253-285.

68.

Mishra, C., & Rath, N. (2020). Social solidarity during a pandemic: Through and beyond Durkheimian lens. Social Sciences & Humanities Open, 2(1), 100079.

69.

Miyachi, T., Takita, M., Senoo, Y., & Yamamoto, K. (2020). Lower trust in national government links to no history of vaccination. The Lancet, 395(10217), 31-32.

70.

Muurinen, C., Laine, M., Pentti, J., Virtanen, M., Salo, P., Kivimäki, M., Vahtera, J., & Oksanen, T. (2014). Vertical and horizontal trust at work as predictors of retirement intentions: The Finnish public sector study. PLOS ONE, 9(9), e106956.

71.

Negură, P., Gașper, L., & Potoroacă, M. (2021). Trust in institutions, social solidarity, and the perception of social cohesion in the Republic of Moldova in the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic. Comparative Southeast European Studies, 69(4), 453-481.

72.

Ozduzen, O., Aslan Ozgul, B., & Ianosev, B. (2023). ‘Institutions of governance are all corrupted’: Anti-political collective identity of anti-lockdown protesters in digital and physical spaces. Social Movement Studies, 23(6), 676-694.

73.

Pires, C. (2022). Global predictors of COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: A systematic review. Vaccines, 10(8), 1349.

74.

Prickett, K. C., & Chapple, S. (2021). Trust in government and Covid-19 vaccine hesitancy. Policy Quarterly, 17(3), 69-71.

75.

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American Community. Touchstone Books by Simon & Schuster.

76.

Qin, C., Liu, Q., Du, M., Yan, W., Tao, L., Wang, Y., Liu, M., & Liu, J. (2022). Neighborhood social cohesion is associated with the willingness toward the booster dose of COVID-19 vaccines among the Chinese older population. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics, 18(6), 2140530.

77.

Ratcliffe, P., & Newman, I. (2011). Promoting social cohesion: Implications for policy and evaluation. Policy Press.

78.

Rogers, A. A., Cook, R. E., & Button, J. A. (2021). Parent and peer norms are unique correlates of COVID-19 vaccine intentions in a diverse sample of U.S. adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health : Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 69(6), 910-916.

79.

Romley, J., Goutam, P., & Sood, N. (2016). National survey indicates that individual vaccination decisions respond positively to community vaccination rates. PLOS ONE, 11(11), e0166858.

80.

Rönnerstrand, B., & Andersson Sundell, K. (2015). Trust, reciprocity and collective action to fight antibiotic resistance. An experimental approach. Social Science & Medicine, 142, 249-255.

81.

Rothbaum, J., & Bee, A. (2021). Coronavirus infects surveys, too: Survey nonresponse bias and the Coronavirus pandemic. US Census Bureau.

82.

Rzymski, P., Mamzer, H., & Nowicki, M. (2021). The main sources and potential effects of COVID-19-related discrimination. In N. Rezaei (Ed.), Coronavirus disease - COVID-19 (pp. 705-725). Springer International.

83.

Saltzman, L. Y., Lesen, A. E., Henry, V., Hansel, T. C., & Bordnick, P. S. (2021). COVID-19 mental health disparities. Health Security, 19(S1), S-5-S-13.

84.

Sato, R., & Takasaki, Y. (2019). Peer effects on vaccination behavior: Experimental evidence from rural Nigeria. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 68(1), 93-129.

85.

Schaurer, I., & Weiß, B. (2020). Investigating selection bias of online surveys on coronavirus-related behavioral outcomes. Survey Research Methods, 14(2), 103-108.

86.

Schoenfeld, E., & Meštrović, S. G. (1989). Durkheim’s concept of justice and its relationship to social solidarity. Sociology of Religion, 50(2), 111-127.

87.

Selvarajah, S., Corona Maioli, S., Deivanayagam, T. A., de Morais Sato, P., Devakumar, D., Kim, S. S., Wells, J. C., Yoseph, M., Abubakar, I., & Paradies, Y. (2022). Racism, xenophobia, and discrimination: Mapping pathways to health outcomes. The Lancet, 400(10368), 2109-2124.

88.

Short, S. E., & Mollborn, S. (2015). Social determinants and health behaviors: Conceptual frames and empirical advances. Current Opinion in Psychology, 5, 78-84.

89.

Shroff, Z. C., Javadi, D., Gilson, L., Kang, R., & Ghaffar, A. (2017). Institutional capacity to generate and use evidence in LMICs: Current state and opportunities for HPSR. Health Research Policy and Systems, 15(1), 94.

90.

Sibicky, M., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). Stigma of psychological therapy: Stereotypes, interpersonal reactions, and the self-fulfilling prophecy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33(2), 148-154.

91.

Siegrist, M., & Cvetkovich, G. (2000). Perception of hazards: The role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Analysis, 20(5), 713-720.

92.

Siegrist, M., Cvetkovich, G., & Roth, C. (2002). Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Analysis, 20(3), 353-362.

93.

Smith, C., & Sorrell, K. (2014). On social solidarity. In V. Jeffries (Ed.), The Palgrave handbook of altruism, morality, and social solidarity (pp. 219-247). Palgrave Macmillan.

94.

Stefaniak, A., Wohl, M. J. A., & Elgar, F. J. (2022). Commentary on “different roles of interpersonal trust and institutional trust in COVID-19 pandemic control.” Social Science & Medicine, 299, 114765.

95.

Sztompka, P. (2006). New perspectives on trust. American Journal of Sociology, 112(3), 905-919.

96.

Tackenberg, B., & Lukas, T. (2019). Resilience through social cohesion: A case study on the role of organizations. In B. Rampp, M. Endreß, & M. Naumann (Eds.), Resilience in social, cultural and political spheres (pp. 323-344). Springer Fachmedien.

97.

Tassier, T., Polgreen, P., & Segre, A. (2015). Vaccination games with peer effects in a heterogeneous hospital worker population. Administrative Sciences, 5(1), 2-26.

98.

Toshkov, D. (2023). Explaining the gender gap in COVID-19 vaccination attitudes. European Journal of Public Health, 33(3), 490-495.

99.

Uslaner, E. M. (2008). The foundations of trust: Macro and micro. Cambridge Journal of Economics, 32(2), 289-294.

100.

Wang, Q., Yang, L., Jin, H., & Lin, L. (2021a). Vaccination against COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis of acceptability and its predictors. Preventive Medicine, 150, 106694.

101.

Wang, S., Berbekova, A., & Uysal, M. M. (2021b). Is this about feeling? The interplay of emotional well-being, solidarity, and residents’ attitude. Journal of Travel Research, 60(6), 1180-1195.

102.

Welch, M. R., Rivera, R. E. N., Conway, B. P., Yonkoski, J., Lupton, P. M., & Giancola, R. (2005). Determinants and consequences of social trust. Sociological Inquiry, 75(4), 453-473.

103.

White, J., Greene, G., Dunstan, F., Rodgers, S., Lyons, R. A., Humphreys, I., John, A., Webster, C., Palmer, S., Elliott, E., Phillips, C. J., & Fone, D. (2014). The communities first (ComFi) study: Protocol for a prospective controlled quasi-experimental study to evaluate the impact of area-wide regeneration on mental health and social cohesion in deprived communities. BMJ Open, 4(10), e006530.

104.

Williams, A. J., Maguire, K., Morrissey, K., Taylor, T., & Wyatt, K. (2020). Social cohesion, mental wellbeing and health-related quality of life among a cohort of social housing residents in Cornwall: A cross sectional study. BMC Public Health, 20(1), 985.

105.

Wu, C., Bierman, A., & Schieman, S. (2022). Socioeconomic stratification and trajectories of social trust during COVID-19. Social Science Research, 108, 102750.

106.

Yu, J., Boyle, E. H., Zhang, Y., Grace, K., & Sangli, G. (2023). Trust and COVID precautionary measures during the early days of the COVID-19 pandemic: Evidence from two African countries. SSM - Population Health, 22, 101397.

107.

Zizumbo-Colunga, D. (2019). Confronting crime by ourselves: Trust in neighbors, trust in authorities, and anti-crime organization. Latin American Research Review, 54(3), 574-590.

Appendices

Appendices
jps-40-2-87-g4
Appendix 1. Data cleaning process
Download Original Figure
Appendix 2. Number of missing values by country Countries included
ID Country 2018 / 2020 2020
No. of records Trust in neighbors Trust in government Age Education No. of records Vaccine acceptance COVID-19 impact on work
1 Argentina 2,001 33 32 4 2 1,001 46 1
2 Australia 2,004 73 44 55 24 1,001 26 1,001
3 Austria 2,000 11 15 3 6 1,000 10 2
4 Bangladesh 2,011 24 106 30 8 1,011 45 1
5 Belgium 2,005 25 24 0 2 1,001 63 4
6 Benin 2,007 79 181 26 13 1,007 81 6
7 Bolivia 2,002 29 83 2 17 1,002 53 5
8 Bosnia and Herzegovina 2,002 29 39 1 6 1,002 87 0
9 Brazil 2,000 94 65 4 16 1,000 22 1
10 Bulgaria 2,008 62 63 2 7 1,007 55 3
11 Burkina Faso 2,002 89 163 21 19 1,002 33 10
12 Cambodia 2,000 100 1,096 2 6 1,000 75 1
13 Cameroon 2,006 76 198 18 22 1,006 14 2
14 Canada 2,022 64 35 26 7 1,010 19 1
15 Chile 2,021 34 38 3 11 1,021 49 1
16 Colombia 2,000 28 32 3 13 1,000 8 0
17 Costa Rica 2,001 22 47 9 8 1,001 44 4
18 Cote d’Ivoire 2,005 127 306 13 20 1,005 44 3
19 Croatia 2,000 39 47 9 9 1,000 115 4
20 Czech Republic 2,000 81 85 17 4 1,000 93 1
21 Denmark 2,000 32 13 0 14 1,000 13 1
22 Dominican Republic 2,000 22 49 4 46 1,000 22 5
23 Ecuador 2,000 38 52 3 22 1,000 44 3
24 Egypt 2,004 33 1,092 5 0 1,004 16 0
25 El Salvador 2,000 27 46 2 0 1,000 32 1
26 Estonia 2,013 146 157 0 1 1,013 82 0
27 Finland 2,000 35 14 0 4 1,000 33 1
28 France 2,000 42 28 4 2 1,000 34 3
29 Gabon 2,005 74 226 34 67 1,005 50 4
30 Georgia 2,000 25 62 2 3 1,000 45 1
31 Germany 2,000 24 22 4 6 1,000 22 1
32 Ghana 2,000 52 86 26 17 1,000 37 4
33 Greece 2,006 38 33 6 4 1,006 46 2
34 Hungary 2,000 34 99 10 5 1,000 87 1
35 India 6,045 112 213 0 37 3,045 39 57
36 Indonesia 2,023 118 214 1 2 1,023 22 4
37 Iraq 2,009 25 45 2 2 1,009 24 1
38 Ireland 2,000 17 29 14 10 1,000 31 2
39 Israel 2,073 75 37 11 3 1,063 129 4
40 Italy 2,000 31 5 1 5 1,000 24 2
41 Japan 2,016 139 210 1 16 1,012 24 1,012
42 Jordan 2,007 46 113 2 5 1,005 63 2
43 Kazakhstan 2,000 62 138 24 15 1,000 146 10
44 Kenya 2,002 25 48 17 4 1,002 13 2
45 Kyrgyz Republic 2,000 64 142 7 13 1,000 133 6
46 Lao PDR 2,001 170 1,153 1 4 1,000 70 11
47 Latvia 2,026 110 149 0 5 1,005 94 4
48 Lebanon 2,035 33 122 3 1 1,035 61 0
49 Lithuania 2,001 169 220 81 87 1,001 249 19
50 Malaysia 2,004 140 255 7 11 1,004 46 6
51 Mali 2,002 56 160 32 18 1,002 23 5
52 Malta 2,009 60 103 1 5 1,002 40 0
53 Mauritius 2,000 32 55 1 3 1,000 19 0
54 Mexico 2,034 27 31 13 7 1,000 47 2
55 Moldova 2,005 49 100 3 2 1,005 1,005 6
56 Mongolia 2,000 99 123 1 1 1,000 63 0
57 Myanmar 2,000 27 116 0 1 1,000 0 0
58 Namibia 2,012 55 105 26 23 1,007 29 1
59 Nepal 2,000 75 262 0 2 1,000 5 2
60 Netherlands 2,001 23 6 3 14 1,000 20 1
61 New Zealand 2,002 60 58 62 8 1,000 1,000 1,000
62 Nicaragua 2,000 21 107 2 23 1,000 23 3
63 Nigeria 2,002 21 49 24 9 1,002 44 5
64 Norway 2,000 46 44 1 36 1,000 18 3
65 Paraguay 2,000 24 54 8 13 1,000 88 10
66 Peru 2,001 32 43 3 21 1,001 8 0
67 Philippines 2,000 5 5 1 0 1,000 18 0
68 Poland 2,002 28 36 25 7 1,002 56 4
69 Portugal 2,005 62 42 7 2 1,004 63 4
70 Republic of Korea 2,023 94 48 4 5 1,009 15 1
71 Romania 2,008 33 72 13 8 1,006 135 14
72 Saudi Arabia 2,029 71 2,029 6 2 1,013 6 1
73 Senegal 2,025 54 144 12 26 1,025 43 3
74 Serbia 2,000 23 107 8 4 1,000 163 2
75 Slovak Republic 2,004 16 18 3 3 1,004 60 4
76 Slovenia 2,001 17 25 18 6 1,001 31 0
77 South Africa 2,004 26 78 18 17 1,004 28 1
78 Spain 2,000 9 27 1 4 1,000 17 2
79 Sri Lanka 2,120 23 164 0 5 1,011 37 2
80 Sweden 2,000 63 25 1 14 1,000 13 1
81 Switzerland 2,000 16 16 4 10 1,000 74 1
82 Taiwan 2,000 88 89 5 3 1,000 53 2
83 Tajikistan 2,000 40 2,000 0 0 1,000 78 3
84 Tanzania 2,000 28 78 6 3 1,000 11 1
85 Thailand 2,000 91 123 0 9 1,000 54 2
86 Turkey 2,000 113 233 6 1 1,000 49 1
87 Uganda 2,027 38 115 10 11 1,027 26 3
88 United Arab Emirates 2,007 197 2,007 7 42 1,002 69 27
89 United Kingdom 2,000 17 27 10 20 1,000 47 3
90 United States 2,007 71 41 9 10 1,001 13 1
91 Uruguay 2,003 61 40 3 4 1,003 51 1
92 Viet Nam 2,012 134 2,012 7 15 1,000 58 6
93 Zambia 2,005 41 98 12 6 1,005 22 1
94 Zimbabwe 2,002 11 121 7 5 1,002 14 1

PDR, proliferative diabetic retinopathy.

Download Excel Table
Countries excluded
ID Country No. of records Reasons for exclusion
2018 2020 Single survey round Income variable not available Google mobility reports not available COVID-19 deaths data not available
1 Afghanistan 1,000 X
2 Albania 1,000 1,000 X
3 Algeria 1,000 1,020 X
4 Armenia 1,000 X
5 Azerbaijan 1,000 X
6 Bahrain 1,005 X
7 Belarus 1,061 X
8 Botswana 1,002 X
9 Burundi 1,000 X
10 Chad 1,000 X X
11 China 3,649 3,502
12 Comoros 1,000 X
13 Cyprus 1,011 1,012 X
14 Eswatini 1,000 X
15 Ethiopia 1,000 1,003 X
16 Gambia 1,000 X
17 Guatemala 1,000 X
18 Guinea 1,000 1,009 X
19 Haiti 500 X
20 Honduras 1,000 X
21 Hong Kong 1,004 X
22 Iceland 500 X
23 Iran 1,005 1,007 X
24 Kosovo 1,000 1,004 X
25 Kuwait 1,001 X
26 Liberia 1,000 X
27 Libya 1,003 X
28 Luxembourg 1,000 X
29 Macedonia 1,008 X
30 Madagascar 1,000 X
31 Malawi 1,000 X
32 Mauritania 1,000 X
33 Montenegro 1,000 1,027 X X
34 Morocco 1,001 1,012 X
35 Mozambique 1,000 X
36 Niger 1,000 X
37 North Macedonia 1,019 X
38 Northern Cyprus 1,000 X
39 Pakistan 1,000 X
40 Palestinian Territories 1,000 X
41 Panama 1,000 X
42 Republic of Congo 1,000 1,009 X
43 Russia 2,000 2,002 X
44 Rwanda 1,000 X
45 Sierra Leone 1,000 X
46 Singapore 1,000 X
47 Togo 1,000 X
48 Tunisia 1,001 1,006 X
49 Turkmenistan 1,000 X
50 Ukraine 1,000 1,000 X
51 Uzbekistan 1,000 1,000 X
52 Venezuela 1,000 1,000 X
53 Yemen 1,000 X
Download Excel Table
Appendix 3. Unweighted sample characteristics with imputation
Variable Without vaccine acceptance (weighted) With vaccine acceptance (weighted)
2018 (n, %) 2020 (n, %) 2020 (n, %)
Size of sample 96,272 96,447 88,414
Trust in neighbors 76.89 74.01 73.62
Trust in government 56.25 59.39 58.79
Vaccine acceptance - - 64.20
Work impacted by COVID-19 - - 50.41
Female 53.21 49.57 49.86
Age
 <25 17.39 19.74 20.09
 25–34 20.62 25.38 25.17
 35–44 17.57 18.88 19.04
 45–54 14.87 13.74 13.99
 55–64 13.27 10.50 10.60
 65+ 16.27 11.77 11.11
Education
 Elementary or less (8 years or less) 26.78 12.46 12.93
 Secondary (8–15 years) 54.00 55.94 56.54
 Tertiary (16+ years) 19.22 31.60 30.53
Income
 Poorest 20% 16.65 14.98 14.87
 Second 20% 18.03 16.68 16.47
 Middle 20% 19.56 19.12 18.98
 Fourth 20% 20.97 21.84 21.92
 Richest 20% 24.79 27.38 27.75
Download Excel Table
Appendix 4. Weighted sample characteristics without imputation
Variables 2018 (n, %) 2020 (n, %)
Sample size 83,693 85,225
Trust in neighbors 79.37 72.20
Trust in government 61.95 63.18
Female 49.79 48.03
Age
 <25 22.47 24.77
 25–34 21.20 23.67
 35–44 18.39 18.99
 45–54 15.11 14.95
 55–64 11.62 8.63
 65+ 11.21 8.98
Education
 Elementary or less (8 years or less) 36.92 33.22
 Secondary (8–15 years) 50.78 51.96
 Tertiary (16+ years) 12.30 14.83
Income
 Poorest 20% 19.76 19.62
 Second 20% 20.06 19.96
 Middle 20% 19.92 20.19
 Fourth 20% 20.13 20.10
 Richest 20% 20.12 20.13
Download Excel Table
Appendix 5. A replication of Table 2 without imputation (weighted)
Variables Beta 95% CI
Trust in neighbors 0.049*** [0.04, 0.06]
Trust in government 0.061* [0.01, 0.11]
Work impacted by COVID-19 0.045* [0.01, 0.08]
Education (ref: under secondary)
 Secondary –0.012 [–0.03, 0.01]
 Tertiary or higher 0.018 [–0.01, 0.05]
Female –0.021*** [–0.03, –0.01]
Age (ref: under 25)
 25–34 –0.016 [–0.06, 0.03]
 35–44 –0.022 [–0.06, 0.02]
 45–54 –0.002 [–0.05, 0.04]
 55–64 0.040** [0.01, 0.07]
 65+ 0.077** [0.03, 0.13]
Income (ref: poorest 20%)
 Second 20% 0.023 [–0.01, 0.05]
 Middle 20% –0.003 [–0.02, 0.02]
 Fourth 20% –0.007 [–0.03, 0.01]
 Richest 20% 0.011 [–0.01, 0.04]
Country-level factors
 Mobility –0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 No. of COVID-19 confirmed deaths 0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 Logged population 0.042* [0.00, 0.08]
 Logged GDP –0.010 [–0.04, 0.02]
Constant –0.105 [–0.69, 0.48]
Observations 78,121

95% confidence intervals in brackets.

* p<0.05,

** p<0.01,

*** p<0.001.

CIs, confidence intervals; GDP, gross domestic product.

Download Excel Table
Appendix 6. Predictors of trust in neighbors in 2018 and 2020 (weighted, full regression output of Fig. 3)
Variables Beta 95% CI
2020 survey round (ref: 2018) –0.169** [–0.30, –0.04]
Country income category (ref: low-income)
 Lower-middle income –0.090 [–0.19, 0.01]
 Upper-middle income –0.150* [–0.27, –0.03]
 High income –0.091 [–0.26, 0.07]
Interactions
 2020 × Lower-middle 0.065 [–0.07, 0.20]
 2020 × Upper-middle 0.131 [–0.00, 0.26]
 2020 × High income 0.180** [0.05, 0.31]
Trust in government 0.175*** [0.14, 0.21]
Education (ref: under secondary)
Secondary 0.029 [–0.01, 0.07]
Tertiary or higher 0.067*** [0.04, 0.10]
Female –0.031*** [–0.04, –0.02]
Age (ref: under 25)
 25–34 0.008 [–0.00, 0.02]
 35–44 0.040*** [0.02, 0.06]
 45–54 0.063*** [0.05, 0.08]
 55–64 0.057*** [0.04, 0.08]
 65+ 0.094*** [0.07, 0.12]
Income (ref: poorest 20%)
 Second 20% 0.020 [–0.01, 0.04]
 Middle 20% 0.030** [0.01, 0.05]
 Fourth 20% 0.031** [0.01, 0.05]
 Richest 20% 0.030** [0.01, 0.05]
Country-level factors
 Mobility 0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 No. of COVID-19 confirmed deaths –0.000 [–0.00, 0.00]
 Logged population –0.019 [–0.06, 0.03]
 Logged GDP 0.027 [–0.01, 0.07]
Constant 0.882** [0.24, 1.52]
Observations 192,719

95% confidence intervals in brackets.

* p<0.05,

** p<0.01,

*** p<0.001.

CIs, confidence intervals; GDP, gross domestic product.

Download Excel Table
jps-40-2-87-g5
Appendix 7. A replication of Fig. 3 without imputation (weighted) Markers indicate the difference in the probability of trust in neighbors between 2018 and 2020 based on multilevel regression. Vertical spikes indicate 95% CIs. Control variables included but not shown: trust in government, the COVID-19 pandemic’s impact on work, being female, age, education, household income, mobility change compared to the baseline period, cumulative COVID-19 confirmed deaths, logged population, and logged GDP per capita. CIs, confidence intervals; GDP, gross domestic product.
Download Original Figure